Image Error: Please email yichiachen@g.ucla.edu with the message 'IMG-ERROR' to receive credit.
What is your job in this experiment?

Drag figure horizontally until it looks good to you.

You have completed the first part!

The second part will take about 2 minutes. You will read 5 simple statements one by one, and rate your agreement.

Hit SPACE to start!

0% completed
You are almost done!
Please answer these last 4 questions:

Did you complete this experiment seriously throughout (without responding randomly)?

You will receive credit regardless of what your answer is here. Please be honest for science! Thanks!

Please answer this question to complete the experiment. Thank you!

Was the browser in the full-screen mode during all the trials?

You will receive credit regardless of what your answer is here. Please be honest for science! Thanks!

Please answer this question to complete the experiment. Thank you!

Was any part of the procedure unclear? Did you have any problems completing any of the tasks?

You will receive credit regardless of what your answer is here. Please be honest for science! Thanks!

Please answer this question to complete the experiment. Thank you!

What is your gender?

Please answer this question to complete the experiment. Thank you!

What is your age?

Please give a valid answer for this question. Thank you!

Thank you very much!
Now, the last thing to do is to hit END to submit your work.

If you are interested in what we are doing, see below for more information!

Debriefing Sheet:


Whenever you pick up your phone to take a picture, you face an aesthetic task that artists faced for thousands of years: How to frame a picture? Besides the famous rule of thirds and golden ratio ideas, there is actually one very robust but undiscussed preference: an inward bias. We like to frame an image in a way that makes the figure in it face inward to the center of the image, leaving more space in its front than its back, rather than outward to the frame borders.

What we want to know in this study is why this is the case. We suspect that this preference has evolved to help us “frame” the world with our visual field--when we turn our head to a particular direction to look at a certain object, we are effectively “framing” the visual world. An inward bias can thus serve an adaptive function: leaving more space in the front of a figure allows us to see what is going to happen next if the figure moves, or is interacted with in the future (since most things tend to happen in the front rather than in the back of a person or an object). To test this idea, we measure your social tendency, which is associated with how readily you make predictions about future movements of social agents, and measure its association with the extent of your inward bias!

Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or concerns about this experiment.

Yi-Chia Chen & Setareh Khoylow
UCLA Computational Vision and Learning Laboratory



Here is some additional information about the bigger project this study is a part of:

We are studying human cognition by looking at how people differ in terms of personality, cognitive tendencies, and skills. Here are a few example scientific questions we are looking at:
- Do people agree what looks pretty and what looks bad?
- How is one’s personality related to their judgement of social situations?
- Would cultural backgrounds influence memory of different objects?